I recently got into a discussion about whether a soft tip actually results in more spin. The best pool physics resource I know of at colostate says that this is a myth:
http://billiards.colostate.edu/threads/cue_tip.html#hardness
I understand the physics of the argument, but I'm still not convinced. First of all, my personal experience says otherwise. If I hit the same shot, same stroke, I get noticeably more draw with a softer tip. One can argue that maybe if I had a better stroke this wouldn't happen, but even if that is the case, it still means that the tip hardness does affect spin. I fully agree that personal experience doesn't mean much scientifically. But it doesn't mean nothing. And I'm not the only person who has experienced this -- most experienced players I've spoken to agree with this assessment.
The claim that hardness doesn't affect spin is based on a model that treats the contact between tip and ball as single instantaneous impulse, where tip hardness only affects the coefficient of restitution, and the duration of contact, but not the direction of the force applied. OK, this is a plausible model, and it is true that the tip and ball don't stay in contact for very long, but still, this isn't what actually happens. What actually happens is that the tip deforms (and the shaft bends slightly), and the force applied to the ball is primarily the result of the tip resisting this deformation and returning to its original shape.
Like I said, modelling the contact as instantaneous might turn out to be accurate, but this is by no means obvious, at least not to me. The tip is a strangely-shaped thing, thicker in the middle than on the sides, and for an off-center hit, it doesn't deform straight back, it deforms a little bit sideways due to the friction and the fact that the ball surface is at an angle.
To really figure out what's going on would require a detailed computer model of the compression of the tip. This is a complicated thing, and it's not clear offhand what the results would be. For example, I'm not even certain that the force vector from the tip to the ball would point in roughly the same direction throughout the contact.
One way to think about problems like this is to do extreme thought experiments. As it happens, the colostate page links to an actual experiment where someone made a tip out of a rubber super ball. The results were much more english (and also much more deflection).
http://www.waynesthisandthat.com/billiards.htm#super tip
Obviously, a soft leather tip is not the same as a rubber tip, but the rubber experiment does show that, with some parameters, the instantaneous impulse model does indeed fall apart. So the question is, do whatever effects that occur with a rubber tip also occur with a soft leather tip to a lesser degree.
I'd be interested what the other physics geeks here think about this topic. One thing that I would find convincing, or at least perplexing, would be some kind of controlled robot experiment showing that that tip consistency doesn't affect draw, at least not in the range of normal leather tips.
http://billiards.colostate.edu/threads/cue_tip.html#hardness
I understand the physics of the argument, but I'm still not convinced. First of all, my personal experience says otherwise. If I hit the same shot, same stroke, I get noticeably more draw with a softer tip. One can argue that maybe if I had a better stroke this wouldn't happen, but even if that is the case, it still means that the tip hardness does affect spin. I fully agree that personal experience doesn't mean much scientifically. But it doesn't mean nothing. And I'm not the only person who has experienced this -- most experienced players I've spoken to agree with this assessment.
The claim that hardness doesn't affect spin is based on a model that treats the contact between tip and ball as single instantaneous impulse, where tip hardness only affects the coefficient of restitution, and the duration of contact, but not the direction of the force applied. OK, this is a plausible model, and it is true that the tip and ball don't stay in contact for very long, but still, this isn't what actually happens. What actually happens is that the tip deforms (and the shaft bends slightly), and the force applied to the ball is primarily the result of the tip resisting this deformation and returning to its original shape.
Like I said, modelling the contact as instantaneous might turn out to be accurate, but this is by no means obvious, at least not to me. The tip is a strangely-shaped thing, thicker in the middle than on the sides, and for an off-center hit, it doesn't deform straight back, it deforms a little bit sideways due to the friction and the fact that the ball surface is at an angle.
To really figure out what's going on would require a detailed computer model of the compression of the tip. This is a complicated thing, and it's not clear offhand what the results would be. For example, I'm not even certain that the force vector from the tip to the ball would point in roughly the same direction throughout the contact.
One way to think about problems like this is to do extreme thought experiments. As it happens, the colostate page links to an actual experiment where someone made a tip out of a rubber super ball. The results were much more english (and also much more deflection).
http://www.waynesthisandthat.com/billiards.htm#super tip
Obviously, a soft leather tip is not the same as a rubber tip, but the rubber experiment does show that, with some parameters, the instantaneous impulse model does indeed fall apart. So the question is, do whatever effects that occur with a rubber tip also occur with a soft leather tip to a lesser degree.
I'd be interested what the other physics geeks here think about this topic. One thing that I would find convincing, or at least perplexing, would be some kind of controlled robot experiment showing that that tip consistency doesn't affect draw, at least not in the range of normal leather tips.