But when you sight an alignment with the CB and OB in set positions, for a cut in a given direction, your line of sight is well defined, right? And after you pivot with a given bridge length (or "effective pivot length"), don't you end up with a well-defined cue alignment? And don't you end up with a different cue alignment for each of the other 5 initial alignments and pivots? And don't each of these cue alignments create a different cut angle?When you sight an "overlap alignment" it's based on light of sight (vertical line in your field of vision).
...is that you completely don't understand the purpose of this thread.One thing is abundantly clear from this thread...
That's because it's obvious without trying it how it works and whether it will be useful. I've now seen the DVD, and there are absolutely no surprises. The way we've always assumed it works (by approximating aim lines and finishing with feel) is exactly the way it really works. Actually, there is one small surprise: there's less to the systematic part of the system than I thought there would be.cookieman:
I think the big three have spent thousands of hours on forums speaking against it and very little time on a table trying it.
I've watched the DVD and the only thing it makes crystal clear is that "the naysayers" have been right all along. Plenty of people have been talking out their asses here - but they all seem to be CTE users.Neil:
...the naysayers are all talking out their collective arses.
...just watch the DVD, and all will be crystal clear.
That's because it's obvious without trying it how it works and whether it will be useful. I've now seen the DVD, and there are absolutely no surprises. The way we've always assumed it works (by approximating aim lines and finishing with feel) is exactly the way it really works. Actually, there is one small surprise: there's less to the systematic part of the system than I thought there would be.
The ones who have used CTE the most are the ones who understand it the least.
pj
chgo
Again in your mind you decide right and wrong, but trying it at the table, hell why would you bother to do that. Not surprised by this post from you.
Maybe, though off hand, it seems to me that drawing the object ball at, say, half the size of the cue ball (simulating a distance of about three feet) would give the same results. However...With the same aligment and pivot - perspective mitigates your reported "off" results.
If you recreated many of your graphs in 3D...you'd see the vector through the core of th cb changes in order to maintain an alignment with the cb/ob points residing on the same vert line.
Thanks for the images. Around 1997-98, at the height of one of the periodic Houle-storms on RSB, I did a bunch of drawings that wound up looking very similar to the ones you have on dr_dave's site. One of my feelings then was that "center of cue ball" didn't always mean the same thing at each of the stages of some of the pivot systems, particularly the CTE variants. That was mostly driven by Hal's reference to "rotating edges", which seemed to imply a shift in alignment from one step to another, giving a different (rotated) perception of the cue ball center from one step to another.This is a simple pivot system where you aim the center of the CB at the contact/impact point (that sends the OB to the pocket/target), then parallel shift the cue/bridge to the center of the OB and pivot back to the center of the CB and shoot
This post is right on the money.Being able to pocket balls with a system doesn't make it exact. Even if you never missed any ball on the table, that still does not make it exact. Exact is a very specific definition that requires CTE to be mathematically proven before it is considered as such.
If the CTE proponents would just admit that CTE is NOT exact and relies on intuition and feel rather than discrete mathematics, all the "naysayers" would not have anything to criticize about CTE and there would be much less negative publicity about it.
Instead proponents of CTE keep insisting that it is exact and crying foul when "naysayers" tell them that its not.
They also seem to resort to insults and personal attacks far more often than the "naysayers", probably due to frustration in not being able to provide any evidence that CTE is exact.
Honestly, those trying to hype CTE up by saying it is exact are probably hurting Stan's sales because potential customers read your statements and then read the statements by Dave and PJ proving them wrong. If you just passed it off as a system that allows you to pocket balls by making alignment more intuitive, you would be much better off.
Thanks for the images. Around 1997-98, at the height of one of the periodic Houle-storms on RSB, I did a bunch of drawings that wound up looking very similar to the ones you have on dr_dave's site. One of my feelings then was that "center of cue ball" didn't always mean the same thing at each of the stages of some of the pivot systems, particularly the CTE variants. That was mostly driven by Hal's reference to "rotating edges", which seemed to imply a shift in alignment from one step to another, giving a different (rotated) perception of the cue ball center from one step to another.
It never occurred to me to look at the CB_center-to-OB_contact_point line. That was probably because I assumed that the point of the various pivot methods was to find an aim line if you couldn't "visualize" the OB contact point. I mean, if you can accurately visualize the OB contact point from your shooting position, why in the world would you care about anything else? Just do the CB-OB overlap and shoot the damn shot.
The pivot-based methods in essence assert that there is a functional relationship between some easily seen path(s) (e.g., CB center to OB edge line, that line shifted to someplace, etc) and the actual aim line necessary to make the contact points come together (equiv., to drive the center of the cue ball directly through the center of the ghost ball). And that they also assert that this functional relationship is such that the cue can be placed on one of these easily seen lines and then pivoted to the cue ball center (whatever center might actually mean) about some convenient point on the cue that is easily discerned "on the fly", after which the cue will point along the needed aim line.
In any case, thank you again for all of your effort in producing your drawings. They are interesting and useful.
There is a geometric proof for the aim and pivot process. It is very simple and has probably been known to a few people for a long time. I posted the proof on this forum and was "shouted down" by the detractors in typical fashion. The problem with my former post was that there was no illustration. I now have one that can be e mailed but I don't want to put my e mail address on one of these forums.
any ideas?
The pivot length does not matter as much as one would assume. I am confident that the process is geometrically sound and while there is probably a more effective range of pivot lengths, it's the process that is transmitted to the ghost ball that produces a consistent result. I wish I knew how to post the illustration, but cannot get my scanned picture to transfer into a post.
I'm also not going to argue about this as I am just responding to the question originally posted.
I was driven to figure out why these aim and pivot systems worked and responded with my results.
I'm not selling anything nor am I trying to carry out any crusades.